Cuttlefish

The name of the pictureThe name of the pictureThe name of the pictureClash Royale CLAN TAG#URR8PPP















Cuttlefish
Temporal range: Miocene – Recent[1]

PreЄ

Є

O

S

D

C

P

T

J

K

Pg

N










Cuttlefish komodo large.jpg
The giant cuttlefish (Sepia apama), above, is the largest species

Scientific classification e
Kingdom:

Animalia
Phylum:

Mollusca
Class:

Cephalopoda
Superorder:

Decapodiformes
Order:

Sepiida
Zittel, 1895

Suborders and Families

  • †Vasseuriina
    • †Vasseuriidae

    • †Belosepiellidae


  • Sepiina
    • †Belosaepiidae

    • Sepiadariidae

    • Sepiidae


    • Sepiolidae Leach, 1817



Synonyms

  • Sepiolida Fioroni, 1981[2]

Cuttlefish or cuttles[3] are marine molluscs of the order Sepiida. They belong to the class Cephalopoda, which also includes squid, octopuses, and nautiluses. Cuttlefish have a unique internal shell, the cuttlebone.


Cuttlefish have large, W-shaped pupils, eight arms, and two tentacles furnished with denticulated suckers, with which they secure their prey. They generally range in size from 15 to 25 cm (6 to 10 in), with the largest species, Sepia apama, reaching 50 cm (20 in) in mantle length and over 10.5 kg (23 lb) in mass.[4]


Cuttlefish eat small molluscs, crabs, shrimp, fish, octopus, worms, and other cuttlefish. Their predators include dolphins, sharks, fish, seals, seabirds, and other cuttlefish. The average life expectancy of a cuttlefish is about 1–2 years. Recent studies indicate cuttlefish are among the most intelligent invertebrates.[5] Cuttlefish also have one of the largest brain-to-body size ratios of all invertebrates.[5]


The "cuttle" in cuttlefish comes from the Old English name for the species, cudele, which may be cognate with the Old Norse koddi (cushion) and the Middle Low German Kudel (rag).[6] The Greco-Roman world valued the cuttlefish as a source of the unique brown pigment the creature releases from its siphon when it is alarmed. The word for it in both Greek and Latin, sepia, now refers to the reddish-brown color sepia in English.


.mw-parser-output .toclimit-2 .toclevel-1 ul,.mw-parser-output .toclimit-3 .toclevel-2 ul,.mw-parser-output .toclimit-4 .toclevel-3 ul,.mw-parser-output .toclimit-5 .toclevel-4 ul,.mw-parser-output .toclimit-6 .toclevel-5 ul,.mw-parser-output .toclimit-7 .toclevel-6 uldisplay:none



Contents





  • 1 Fossil record


  • 2 Range and habitat


  • 3 Anatomy and physiology

    • 3.1 Visual system


    • 3.2 Circulatory system


    • 3.3 Cuttlebone


    • 3.4 Specialized components

      • 3.4.1 Ink


      • 3.4.2 Suckers and venom




  • 4 Reproduction


  • 5 Communication

    • 5.1 Chromatic

      • 5.1.1 Chromatophores


      • 5.1.2 Iridophores


      • 5.1.3 Leucophores



    • 5.2 Intraspecific communication


    • 5.3 Interspecific communication

      • 5.3.1 Camouflage




  • 6 Diet


  • 7 Taxonomy


  • 8 Human uses

    • 8.1 As food


    • 8.2 Sepia


    • 8.3 Metal casting


    • 8.4 Smart clothing


    • 8.5 Pets



  • 9 See also


  • 10 References


  • 11 External links




Fossil record


The earliest sepia-like fossils of cuttlefish are from the Cretaceous period.[7][8] Whether the earlier Trachyteuthis is assigned to this class, or to the Octopodiformes, remains unclear.[9]



Range and habitat





S. mestus swimming (Australia)


The family Sepiidae, which contains all cuttlefish, inhabits tropical and temperate ocean waters. They are mostly shallow-water animals, although they are known to go to depths of about 600 m (2,000 ft).[10] They have an unusual biogeographic pattern: they are present along the coasts of East and South Asia, Western Europe, and the Mediterranean, as well as all coasts of Africa and Australia, but are totally absent from the Americas. By the time the family evolved, ostensibly in the Old World, the North Atlantic possibly had become too cold and deep for these warm-water species to cross.[11]
The common cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis), is found in the Mediterranean, North, and Baltic Seas, although populations may occur as far south as South Africa. They are found in sublittoral depths, between the low tide line and the edge of the continental shelf, to about 180 m (100 fathoms)."[12] The cuttlefish is listed under the Red List category of "least concern" by the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. This means that while some over-exploitation of the marine animal has occurred in some regions due to large-scale commercial fishing, their wide geographic range prevents them from being too threatened. Ocean acidification, however, caused largely by higher levels of carbon dioxide emitted into the atmosphere, is cited as a potential threat.[13]



Anatomy and physiology



Visual system




The characteristic W-shape of the cuttlefish eye




File:Sepia eyelid shape.theora.ogvPlay media

Pupil expansion in Sepia officinalis


Cuttlefish, like other cephalopods, have sophisticated eyes. The organogenesis and the final structure of the cephalopod eye fundamentally differ from those of vertebrates such as humans.[14]
Superficial similarities between cephalopod and vertebrate eyes are thought to be examples of convergent evolution. The cuttlefish pupil is a smoothly curving W-shape.[15][16] Although cuttlefish cannot see color,[17] they can perceive the polarization of light, which enhances their perception of contrast. They have two spots of concentrated sensor cells on their retinas (known as foveae), one to look more forward, and one to look more backward. The eye changes focus by shifting the position of the entire lens with respect to the retina, instead of reshaping the lens as in mammals. Unlike the vertebrate eye, no blind spot exists, because the optic nerve is positioned below the retina. This allows cuttlefish to see in front of them and behind them at the same time.


The cuttlefish's eyes are thought to be fully developed before birth, and they start observing their surroundings while still in the egg. In consequence, they may prefer to hunt the prey they saw before hatching.[18]



Circulatory system


The blood of a cuttlefish is an unusual shade of green-blue, because it uses the copper-containing protein haemocyanin to carry oxygen instead of the red, iron-containing protein haemoglobin found in vertebrates' blood. The blood is pumped by three separate hearts: two branchial hearts pump blood to the cuttlefish's pair of gills (one heart for each), and the third pumps blood around the rest of the body. Cuttlefish blood must flow more rapidly than that of most other animals because haemocyanin carries substantially less oxygen than haemoglobin. Unlike most other mollusks, cephalopods like cuttlefish have a closed circulatory system.



Cuttlebone



@media all and (max-width:720px).mw-parser-output .tmulti>.thumbinnerwidth:100%!important;max-width:none!important.mw-parser-output .tmulti .tsinglefloat:none!important;max-width:none!important;width:100%!important;text-align:center




Top and bottom view of a cuttlebone, the buoyancy organ and internal shell of a cuttlefish


Cuttlefish possess an internal structure called the cuttlebone, which is porous and is made of aragonite. The pores provide it with buoyancy, which the cuttlefish regulates by changing the gas-to-liquid ratio in the chambered cuttlebone via the ventral siphuncle.[19] Each species' cuttlebone has a distinct shape, size, and pattern of ridges or texture. The cuttlebone is unique to cuttlefish, and is one of the features that distinguish them from their squid relatives.[20]



Specialized components



Ink


Like other marine mollusks, cuttlefish have ink stores that are used for chemical deterrence, phagomimicry, sensory distraction, and evasion when attacked.[21] The composition of the ink leads to a richly colored "black" ink which is rich in ammonium salts and amino acids that may have a role in phagomimicry defenses.[21]


The use of this color is wide-ranged. A common use is in cooking with squid ink to darken and flavor rice and pasta.[citation needed] It is purported to add a sweet flavor to the food, and leaves the food stained a dark black color. In addition to food, cuttlefish ink can be used with plastics and staining of materials.[citation needed] The diverse composition of cuttlefish ink, and its deep complexity of colors, allows for dilution and modification of its color. Cuttlefish ink can be used to make noniridescent reds, blues, and greens,
[22] subsequently used for biomimetic colors and materials.[citation needed]



Suckers and venom


The suckers of cuttlefish extend most of the length of their arms and along the distal portion of their tentacles. Like other cephalopods, cuttlefish have "taste-by-touch" sensitivity in their suckers, allowing them to discriminate among objects and water currents that they contact.[23]


Some cuttlefish are venomous. The genes for venom production are thought to be descended from a common ancestor.[24] The muscles of the flamboyant cuttlefish (Metasepia pfefferi) contain a highly toxic, unidentified compound[5] as lethal as that of a fellow cephalopod, the blue-ringed octopus.[25]



Reproduction


Male cuttlefish challenge one another for dominance and the best den during mating season. During this challenge, no direct contact is usually made. The animals threaten each other until one of them backs down and swims away. Eventually, the larger male cuttlefish mate with the females by grabbing them with their tentacles, turning the female so that the two animals are face-to-face, then using a specialized tentacle to insert sperm sacs into an opening near the female's mouth. The male then guards the female until she lays the eggs a few hours later.[26]


On occasion, a large competitor arrives to threaten the male cuttlefish. In these instances, the male first attempts to intimidate the other male. If the competitor does not flee, the male eventually attacks it to force it away. The cuttlefish that can paralyze the other first, by forcing near its mouth, wins the fight and the female. Since typically four or five (and sometimes as many as 10) males are available for every female, this behavior is inevitable.[27]


Cuttlefish are indeterminate growers, so smaller cuttlefish always have a chance at finding a mate the next year when they are bigger.[28] Additionally, cuttlefish unable to win in a direct confrontation with a guard male have been observed employing several other tactics to acquire a mate. The most successful of these methods is camouflage; smaller cuttlefish use their camouflage abilities to disguise themselves as a female cuttlefish. Changing their body color, and even pretending to be holding an egg sack, disguised males are able to swim past the larger guard male and mate with the female.[27][29][30]



Communication


Cephalopods are able to communicate visually using a diverse range of signals. To produce these signals, cephalopods can vary four types of communication element: chromatic (skin coloration), skin texture (e.g. rough or smooth), posture, and locomotion. Changes in body appearance such as these are sometimes called polyphenism. The common cuttlefish can display 34 chromatic, six textural, eight postural and six locomotor elements, whereas flamboyant cuttlefish use between 42 and 75 chromatic, 14 postural, and seven textural and locomotor elements. For the Caribbean reef squid (Sepioteuthis sepioidea), they are thought to have up to 35 distinct signalling states.[31][32]

































































































Visual signals of the common cuttlefish[31]
Chromic - light
Chromic - dark
Texture
Posture
Locomotor
White posterior triangle
Anterior transverse mantle line
Smooth skin
Raised arms
Sitting
White square
Posterior transverse mantle line
Coarse skin
Waving arms
Bottom suction
White mantle bar
Anterior mantle bar

Papillate skin
Splayed arms
Buried
White lateral stripe
Posterior mantle bar
Wrinkled first arms
Drooping arms
Hovering
White fin spots
Paired mantle spots
White square papillae
Extended fourth arm
Jetting
White fin line
Median mantle stripe
Major lateral papillae
Flattened body
Inking
White neck spots
Mantle margin stripe

Raised head

Iridescent ventral mantle
Mantle margin scalloping

Flanged fin

White zebra bands
Dark fin line



White landmark spots
Black zebra bands



White splotches
Mottle



White major lateral papillae
Lateroventral patches



White head bar
Anterior head bar



White arm triangle
Posterior head bar



Pink iridophore arm stripes
Pupil



White arms spots (males only)
Eye ring



Dark arm stripes




Dark arms





Chromatic



Two cuttlefish with dramatically different coloration

This broadclub cuttlefish (Sepia latimanus) can change from camouflage tans and browns (top) to yellow with dark highlights (bottom) in less than one second.


Cuttlefish are sometimes referred to as the "chameleons of the sea" because of their ability to rapidly alter their skin color – this can occur within one second. Cuttlefish change color and pattern (including the polarization of the reflected light waves), and the shape of the skin to communicate to other cuttlefish, to camouflage themselves, and as a deimatic display to warn off potential predators. Under some circumstances, cuttlefish can be trained to change color in response to stimuli, thereby indicating their color changing is not completely innate.[33]


Cuttlefish can also affect the light's polarization, which can be used to signal to other marine animals, many of which can also sense polarization, as well as being able to influence the color of light as it reflects off their skin.[34] Although cuttlefish (and most other cephalopods) lack color vision, high-resolution polarisation vision may provide an alternative mode of receiving contrast information that is just as defined.[35] The cuttlefish's wide pupil attenuates chromatic aberration, allowing it to perceive color by focusing specific wavelengths onto the retina.[36]


The three broad categories of color patterns are uniform, mottle, and disruptive.[37] Cuttlefish can display as many as 12 to 14 patterns,[31] 13 of which have been categorized as seven "acute" (relatively brief) and six "chronic" (long-lasting) patterns.[38] although other researchers suggest the patterns occur on a continuum.[37]


















Patterns of the common cuttlefish[31]
Chronic
Acute
Uniform light
Uniform blanching
Stipple
Uniform darkening
Light mottle
Acute disruptive
Disruptive
Deimatic
Dark mottle
Flamboyant
Weak zebra
Intense zebra

Passing cloud

The color-changing ability of cuttlefish is due to multiple types of cells. These are arranged (from the skin's surface going deeper) as pigmented chromatophores above a layer of reflective iridophores and below them, leucophores.[39][40]



Chromatophores


The chromatophores are sacs containing hundreds of thousands of pigment granules and a large membrane that is folded when retracted. Hundreds of muscles radiate from the chromatophore. These are under neural control and when they expand, they reveal the hue of the pigment contained in the sac. Cuttlefish have three types of chromatophore: yellow/orange (the uppermost layer), red, and brown/black (the deepest layer). The cuttlefish can control the contraction and relaxation of the muscles around individual chromatophores, thereby opening or closing the elastic sacs and allowing different levels of pigment to be exposed.[32] Furthermore, the chromatophores contain luminescent protein nanostructures in which tethered pigment granules modify light through absorbance, reflection, and fluorescence between 650 and 720 nm.[41][42]


For cephalopods in general, the hues of the pigment granules are relatively constant within a species, but can vary slightly between species. For example, the common cuttlefish and the opalescent inshore squid (Loligo opalescens) have yellow, red, and brown, the European common squid (Alloteuthis subulata) has yellow and red, and the common octopus has yellow, orange, red, brown, and black.[32]


In cuttlefish, activation of a chromatophore can expand its surface area by 500%. Up to 200 chromatophores per mm2 of skin may occur. In Loligo plei, an expanded chromatophore may be up to 1.5 mm in diameter, but when retracted, it can measure as little as 0.1 mm.[41][43][44]



Iridophores


Retracting the chromatophores reveals the iridophores and leucophores beneath them, thereby allowing cuttlefish to use another modality of visual signalling brought about by structural coloration.


Iridophores are structures that produce iridescent colors with a metallic sheen. They reflect light using plates of crystalline chemochromes made from guanine. When illuminated, they reflect iridescent colors because of the diffraction of light within the stacked plates. Orientation of the schemochrome determines the nature of the color observed. By using biochromes as colored filters, iridophores create an optical effect known as Tyndall or Rayleigh scattering, producing bright blue or blue-green colors. Iridophores vary in size, but are generally smaller than 1 mm. Squid at least are able to change their iridescence. This takes several seconds or minutes, and the mechanism is not understood.[45] However, iridescence can also be altered by expanding and retracting the chromatophores above the iridophores. Because chromatophores are under direct neural control from the brain, this effect can be immediate.


Cephalopod iridophores polarize light. Cephalopods have a rhabdomeric visual system which means they are visually sensitive to polarized light. Cuttlefish use their polarization vision when hunting for silvery fish (their scales polarize light). Female cuttlefish exhibit a greater number of polarized light displays than males and also alter their behavior when responding to polarized patterns. The use of polarized reflective patterns has led some to suggest that cephalopods may communicate intraspecifically in a mode that is "hidden" or "private" because many of their predators are insensitive to polarized light.[45][46][44]



Leucophores




The white spots and bands on this cuttlefish are produced by leucophores.


Leucophores, usually located deeper in the skin than iridophores, are also structural reflectors using crystalline purines, often guanine, to reflect light. Unlike iridophores, however, leucophores have more organized crystals that reduce diffraction. Given a source of white light, they produce a white shine, in red they produce red, and in blue they produce blue. Leucophores assist in camouflage by providing light areas during background matching (e.g. by resembling light-colored objects in the environment) and disruptive coloration (by making the body appear to be composed of high-contrasting patches).[45]


The reflectance spectra of cuttlefish patterns and several natural substrates (stipple, mottle, disruptive) can be measured using an optic spectrometer.[45]



Intraspecific communication


Cuttlefish sometimes use their color patterns to signal future intent to other cuttlefish. For example, during agonistic encounters, male cuttlefish adopt a pattern called the intense zebra pattern, considered to be an honest signal. If a male is intending to attack, it adopts a "dark face" change, otherwise, it remains pale.[47]


In at least one species, female cuttlefish react to their own reflection in a mirror and to other females by displaying a body pattern called "splotch". However, they do not use this display in response to males, inanimate objects, or prey. This indicates they are able to discriminate same-sex conspecifics, even when human observers are unable to discern the sex of a cuttlefish in the absence of sexual dimorphism.[48]


Female cuttlefish signal their receptivity to mating using a display called precopulatory grey.[48] Male cuttlefish sometimes use deception toward guarding males to mate with females. Small males hide their sexually dimorphic fourth arms, change their skin pattern to the mottled appearance of females, and change the shape of their arms to mimic those of nonreceptive, egg-laying females.[30]


Displays on one side of a cuttlefish can be independent of the other side of the body; males can display courtship signals to females on one side while simultaneously showing female-like displays with the other side to stop rival males interfering with their courtship.[49]



Interspecific communication


The deimatic display (a rapid change to black and white with dark ‘eyespots’ and contour, and spreading of the body and fins) is used to startle small fish that are unlikely to prey on the cuttlefish, but use the flamboyant display towards larger, more dangerous fish,[50] and give no display at all to chemosensory predators such as crabs and dogfish.[51]


One dynamic pattern shown by cuttlefish is dark mottled waves apparently repeatedly moving down the body of the animals. This has been called the passing cloud pattern. In the common cuttlefish, this is primarily observed during hunting, and is thought to communicate to potential prey – “stop and watch me”[32] – which some have interpreted as a type of "hypnosis".



Camouflage





Juvenile cuttlefish camouflaged against the seafloor



External video

Kings of CamouflageNova documentary

Cuttlefish are able to rapidly change the color of their skin to match their surroundings and create chromatically complex patterns,[51] despite their inability to perceive color, through some mechanism which is not completely understood.[52] They have been seen to have the ability to assess their surroundings and match the color, contrast and texture of the substrate even in nearly total darkness.[43]


The color variations in the mimicked substrate and animal skin are similar. Depending on the species, the skin of cuttlefish responds to substrate changes in distinctive ways. By changing naturalistic backgrounds, the camouflage responses of different species can be measured.[53]Sepia officinalis changes color to match the substrate by disruptive patterning (contrast to break up the outline), whereas S. pharaonis matches the substrate by blending in. Although camouflage is achieved in different ways, and in an absence of color vision, both species change their skin colors to match the substrate. Cuttlefish adapt their own camouflage pattern in ways that are specific for a particular habitat. An animal could settle in the sand and appear one way, with another animal a few feet away in a slightly different microhabitat, settled in algae for example, will be camouflaged quite differently.[43]


Cuttlefish are also able to change the texture of their skin. The skin contains bands of circular muscle which as they contract, push fluid up. These can be seen as little spikes, bumps, or flat blades. This can help with camouflage when the cuttlefish becomes texturally as well as chromatically similar to objects in its environment such as kelp or rocks.[43]




Diet




File:Red cuttle hunting.webmPlay media

Video of S. mestus in Sydney waters, hunting and catching prey


While the preferred diet of cuttlefish is crabs and fish, they feed on small shrimp shortly after hatching.[54]


Cuttlefish use their camouflage to hunt and sneak up on their prey.[55] They swim at the bottom, where shrimp and crabs are found, and shoot out a jet of water to uncover the prey buried in the sand. Then when the prey tries to escape, the cuttlefish open their eight arms and shoot out two long feeding tentacles to grab them. Each arm has a pad covered in suckers, which grabs and pulls prey toward its beak, paralyzing it with venom before eating it.[54] To achieve a hypnotic effect and stun prey before catching it, cuttlefish are also known to change color rapidly.[citation needed]



Taxonomy






Illustration of Sepia officinalis




File:Cuttlefish.ogvPlay media

Video of a cuttlefish in its natural habitat


Over 120 species of cuttlefish are currently recognised, grouped into six families.


  • Order Sepiida: cuttlefish
    • Suborder †Vasseuriina
      • Family †Vasseuriidae

      • Family †Belosepiellidae


    • Suborder Sepiina
      • Family †Belosaepiidae

      • Family Sepiadariidae

      • Family Sepiidae

      • Family Sepiolidae




Human uses



As food



Three-sided white plate containing linguini

Linguine with cuttlefish and ink sauce served at a Venetian osteria


Cuttlefish are caught for food in the Mediterranean, East Asia, the English Channel, and elsewhere.


In East Asia, dried, shredded cuttlefish is a popular snack food. In the Qing Dynasty manual of Chinese gastronomy, the Suiyuan shidan, the roe of the cuttlefish is considered a difficult-to-prepare, but sought-after delicacy.[56]


Cuttlefish are quite popular in Europe. For example, in northeast Italy, they are used in risotto al nero di seppia (risotto with cuttlefish ink), also found in Croatia and Montenegro as crni rižot (black risotto). Catalan cuisine, especially that of the coastal regions, uses cuttlefish and squid ink in a variety of tapas and dishes such as arròs negre. Breaded and deep-fried cuttlefish is a popular dish in Andalusia. In Portugal, cuttlefish is present in many popular dishes. Chocos com tinta (cuttlefish in black ink), for example, is grilled cuttlefish in a sauce of its own ink. Cuttlefish is also popular in the region of Setúbal, where it is served as deep-fried strips or in a variant of feijoada, with white beans. Black pasta is often made using cuttlefish ink.



Sepia


Cuttlefish ink was formerly an important dye, called sepia. Today, artificial dyes have mostly replaced natural sepia.



Metal casting


Cuttlebone has been used since antiquity to make casts for metal. A model is pushed into the cuttlebone and removed, leaving an impression. Molten gold, silver or pewter can then be poured into the cast.[57][58]



Smart clothing


Research into replicating biological color-changing has led to engineering artificial chromatophores out of small devices known as dielectric elastomer actuators. Engineers at the University of Bristol have engineered soft materials that mimic the color-changing skin of animals like cuttlefish,[59] paving the way for "smart clothing" and camouflage applications.[60]



Pets


Though cuttlefish are rarely kept as pets, due in part to their fairly short lifetimes, the most common to be kept are Sepia officinalis or Sepia bandensis.[61]


Cuttlebone is given to parakeets and other cagebirds as a source of dietary calcium.[20]



See also




  • Cephalopod size


References




  1. ^ (see text


  2. ^ Philippe Bouchet (2018). "Sepiida". World Register of Marine Species. Flanders Marine Institute. Retrieved 17 February 2019..mw-parser-output cite.citationfont-style:inherit.mw-parser-output .citation qquotes:"""""""'""'".mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-free abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/6/65/Lock-green.svg/9px-Lock-green.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-limited a,.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-registration abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/d/d6/Lock-gray-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-gray-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-subscription abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/a/aa/Lock-red-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-red-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registrationcolor:#555.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription span,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration spanborder-bottom:1px dotted;cursor:help.mw-parser-output .cs1-ws-icon abackground:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/4/4c/Wikisource-logo.svg/12px-Wikisource-logo.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center.mw-parser-output code.cs1-codecolor:inherit;background:inherit;border:inherit;padding:inherit.mw-parser-output .cs1-hidden-errordisplay:none;font-size:100%.mw-parser-output .cs1-visible-errorfont-size:100%.mw-parser-output .cs1-maintdisplay:none;color:#33aa33;margin-left:0.3em.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration,.mw-parser-output .cs1-formatfont-size:95%.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-left,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-leftpadding-left:0.2em.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-right,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-rightpadding-right:0.2em


  3. ^ "The Cephalopoda". University of California Museum of Paleontology. Retrieved 2017-06-27.


  4. ^ Reid, A., P. Jereb, & C. F. E. Roper (2005). "Family Sepiidae". In: P. Jereb & C. F. E. Roper, eds. Cephalopods of the world. An annotated and illustrated catalogue of species known to date. Volume 1. Chambered nautiluses and sepioids (Nautilidae, Sepiidae, Sepiolidae, Sepiadariidae, Idiosepiidae and Spirulidae). FAO Species Catalogue for Fishery Purposes. No. 4, Vol. 1. Rome, FAO. pp. 57–152.


  5. ^ abc NOVA, 2007. Cuttlefish: Kings of Camouflage. (television program) NOVA, PBS, April 3, 2007.


  6. ^ Stevenson, Angus (20 September 2007). Shorter Oxford English Dictionary. OUP Oxford. p. 3804. ISBN 978-0-19-920687-2.


  7. ^ Whiteaves, J.F. (1897). "On some remains of a Sepia-like cuttle-fish from the Cretaceous rocks of the South Saskatchewan". The Canadian Record of Science. 7: 459–462.


  8. ^ Hewitt, R.; Pedley, H. M. (1978). "The preservation of the shells of Sepia in the middle Miocene of Malta". Proceedings of the Geologists' Association. 89 (3): 227–237. doi:10.1016/S0016-7878(78)80013-3.


  9. ^ Fuchs, Dirk; Stinnesbeck, Wolfgang; Ifrim, Christina; Giersch, Samuel; Padilla Gutierrez, José Manuel; Frey, Eberhard (2010). "Glyphiteuthis rhinophora n. sp., a trachyteuthidid (Coleoidea, Cephalopoda) from the Cenomanian (Late Cretaceous) of Mexico". Paläontologische Zeitschrift. 84 (4): 523–32. doi:10.1007/s12542-010-0066-9.


  10. ^ Lu, C. C. and Roper, C. F. E. (1991). "Aspects of the biology of Sepia cultrata from southeastern Australia", p. 192 in: La Seiche, The Cuttlefish. Boucaud-Camou, E. (Ed). Caen, France; Centre de Publications de l'Université de Caen.


  11. ^ Young, R. E., Vecchione, M. and Donovan, D. (1998). "The evolution of coleoid cephalopods and their present biodiversity and ecology". South African Journal of Marine Science. 20: 393–420. doi:10.2989/025776198784126287.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  12. ^ Common Cuttlefishes, Sepia officinalis. marinebio.org


  13. ^ "Sepia officinalis (Common Cuttlefish)". www.iucnredlist.org. Retrieved 2017-01-12.


  14. ^ Muller, Matthew. "Development of the Eye in Vertebrates and Cephalopods and Its Implications for Retinal Structure". The Cephalopod Eye. Davidson College Biology Department. Retrieved 2007-04-06.
    [dead link]



  15. ^ Schaeffel, F.; Murphy, C. J.; Howland, H. C. (1999). "Accommodation in the cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis)". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (22): 3127–3134. PMID 10539961.


  16. ^ Murphy, C. J.; Howland, H. C. (1990). "The functional significance of crescent-shaped pupils and multiple pupillary apertures". Journal of Experimental Zoology. 256: 22–28. doi:10.1002/jez.1402560505.


  17. ^ Mäthger LM, Barbosa A, Miner S, Hanlon RT (2006). "Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay" (PDF). Vision Research. 46 (11): 1746–53. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.09.035. PMID 16376404. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 7, 2013.


  18. ^ "Cuttlefish spot target prey early". BBC News. 2008-06-05. Retrieved 2008-05-06.


  19. ^ Rexfort, A.; Mutterlose, J. (2006). "Stable isotope records from Sepia officinalis—a key to understanding the ecology of belemnites?". Earth and Planetary Science Letters. 247 (3–4): 212. Bibcode:2006E&PSL.247..212R. doi:10.1016/j.epsl.2006.04.025.


  20. ^ ab Staaf, Danna (2017). Squid Empire: The Rise and Fall of the Cephalopods. University Press of New England. pp. 112–. ISBN 978-1-5126-0128-2.


  21. ^ ab Derby, Charles D.; Kicklighter, Cynthia E.; Johnson, P. M.; Zhang, Xu (2007-05-01). "Chemical Composition of Inks of Diverse Marine Molluscs Suggests Convergent Chemical Defenses". Journal of Chemical Ecology. 33 (5): 1105–1113. doi:10.1007/s10886-007-9279-0. ISSN 0098-0331. PMID 17393278.


  22. ^ Zhang, Yafeng; Dong, Biqin; Chen, Ang; Liu, Xiaohan; Shi, Lei; Zi, Jian (2015). "Using Cuttlefish Ink as an Additive to Produce Non‐iridescent Structural Colors of High-Color Visibility". Advanced Materials. 27 (32): 4719–24. doi:10.1002/adma.201501936. PMID 26175211.


  23. ^ Hanlon, Roger T.; John B. Messenger, John B. (1998). Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. p. 16. ISBN 978-0521645836.


  24. ^ All Octopuses Are Venomous, Study Says. News.nationalgeographic.com (2010-10-28). Retrieved on 2011-09-18.


  25. ^ Teacher's Guide to NOVA episode – Kings of Camouflage on PBS (After Watching: Activity 2).


  26. ^ Bavendam, Fred (1995) "The Giant Cuttlefish Chameleon of the Reef". National Geographic, pp. 94–107. Print.


  27. ^ ab
    Mating Trick: Science Videos. Science News – ScienCentral



  28. ^ Life: Cuttlefish Wards Off Rivals : Video : Discovery Channel. Dsc.discovery.com (2012-03-22). Retrieved on 2013-09-18.


  29. ^ Ebert, Jessica (2005). "Cuttlefish win mates with transvestite antics". News@nature. doi:10.1038/news050117-9.


  30. ^ ab Hanlon, RT; Naud, MJ; Shaw, PW; Havenhand, JN (2005). "Behavioural ecology: Transient sexual mimicry leads to fertilization" (PDF). Nature. 433 (7023): 212. Bibcode:2005Natur.433..212H. doi:10.1038/433212a. PMID 15662403. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 17, 2015.


  31. ^ abcd Crook, A.C., Baddeley, R. and Osorio, D. (2002). "Identifying the structure in cuttlefish visual signals". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 357 (1427): 1617–1624. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1070. PMC 1693061. PMID 12495518.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  32. ^ abcd Thomas, A.; MacDonald, C. (2016). "Investigating body patterning in aquarium-raised flamboyant cuttlefish (Metasepia pfefferi)". PeerJ. 4: e2035. doi:10.7717/peerj.2035. PMC 4878381. PMID 27231657.


  33. ^ Hough, A.R., Case, J. and Boal, J.G. (2016). "Learned control of body patterning in cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda)". Journal of Molluscan Studies. 82 (3): 427–431. doi:10.1093/mollus/eyw006.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  34. ^ Mäthger, L. M., Shashar, N. and Hanlon, R. T. (2009). "Do cephalopods communicate using polarized light reflections from their skin?". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (14): 2133–40. doi:10.1242/jeb.020800. PMID 19561202.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  35. ^ Temple, S.E., Pignatelli, V., Cook, T., How, M.J., Chiou, T.H., Roberts, N.W. and Marshall, N.J. (2012). "High-resolution polarisation vision in a cuttlefish". Current Biology. 22 (4): R121–R122. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.010. PMID 22361145.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  36. ^ Stubbs, A.; Stubbs, C. (2016). "Spectral discrimination in color-blind animals via chromatic aberration and pupil shape". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (29): 8206–8211. doi:10.1073/pnas.1524578113. PMC 4961147. PMID 27382180.


  37. ^ ab Chiao, C.C., Chubb, C., Buresch, K.C., Barbosa, A., Allen, J.J., Mäthger, L.M. and Hanlon, R.T. (2010). "Mottle camouflage patterns in cuttlefish: quantitative characterization and visual background stimuli that evoke them". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 213 (2): 187–199. doi:10.1242/jeb.030247. PMID 20038652.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  38. ^ Hanlon, R.T.; Messenger, J.B. (1988). "Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.): the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behaviour". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 320 (1200): 437–487. Bibcode:1988RSPTB.320..437H. doi:10.1098/rstb.1988.0087. JSTOR 2396667.


  39. ^ Messenger J.B. (2001). "Cephalopod chromatophores: neurobiology and natural history". Biological Reviews. 76 (4): 473–528. doi:10.1017/S1464793101005772. PMID 11762491.


  40. ^ NOVA | Kings of Camouflage | Anatomy of a Cuttlefish (non-Flash). PBS. Retrieved on 2013-09-18.


  41. ^ ab Karoff, P. (2014). ""Chameleon of the sea" reveals its secrets". Harvard. Retrieved May 26, 2014.


  42. ^ Deravi, L.F.; et al. (2014). "The structure–function relationships of a natural nanoscale photonic device in cuttlefish chromatophores". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 11 (93): 20130942. doi:10.1098/rsif.2013.0942. PMC 3928930. PMID 24478280.


  43. ^ abcd Hansford, D. (2008). "Cuttlefish change color, shape-shift to elude predators". National Geographic.


  44. ^ ab Mäthger, L. M.; Denton, E. J.; Marshall, N. J.; Hanlon, R. T. (2009). "Mechanisms and behavioural functions of structural coloration in cephalopods". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 6 Suppl 2: S149–63. doi:10.1098/rsif.2008.0366.focus. PMC 2706477. PMID 19091688.


  45. ^ abcd Mathger, L. M.; Chiao, C.; Barbosa, A. & Hanlon, R. T. (2008). "Color matching on natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 194 (6): 577–85. doi:10.1007/s00359-008-0332-4. PMID 18414874.


  46. ^ Mäthger, L.M., Shashar, N. and Hanlon, R.T. (2009). "Do cephalopods communicate using polarized light reflections from their skin?". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (14): 2133–2140. doi:10.1242/jeb.020800. PMID 19561202.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  47. ^ Adamo, S.A.; Hanlon, R.T. (1996). "Do cuttlefish (Cephalopoda) signal their intentions to conspecifics during agonistic encounters?". Animal Behaviour. 52 (1): 73–81. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0153.


  48. ^ ab Palmer, M.E., Calvé, M.R. and Adamo, S.A. (2006). "Response of female cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda) to mirrors and conspecifics: evidence for signaling in female cuttlefish". Animal Cognition. 9 (2): 151–155. doi:10.1007/s10071-005-0009-0. PMID 16408230.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  49. ^ Hutton, P., Seymoure, B.M., McGraw, K.J., Ligon, R.A. and Simpson, R.K. (2015). "Dynamic color communication". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 6: 41–49. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.08.007.CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link)


  50. ^ Langridge, K.V. (2009). "Cuttlefish use startle displays, but not against large predators". Animal Behaviour. 77 (4): 847–856. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.023.


  51. ^ ab Stuart-Fox, D.; Moussalli, A. (2009). "Camouflage, communication and thermoregulation: Lessons from color changing organisms". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 364 (1516): 463–70. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0254. PMC 2674084. PMID 19000973.


  52. ^ Mäthger, Lydia M.; Barbosa, Alexandra; Miner, Simon; Hanlon, Roger T. (May 2006). "Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay". Vision Research. 46 (11): 1746–1753. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.09.035. PMID 16376404.


  53. ^ Shohet, A.; Baddeley, R.; Anderson, J. & Osorio, D. (2007). "Cuttlefish camouflage: A quantitative study of patterning". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 92 (2): 335–345. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00842.x.


  54. ^ ab Cuttlefish Basics. Tonmo.com (2003-02-12). Retrieved on 2011-09-18.


  55. ^ Cousteau, Jacques-Yves; Diolé, Philippe (1979). Octopus and Squid: The Soft Intelligence. Garden City, N.Y.: Cassell. ISBN 978-0385068963.


  56. ^ "Seafoods 7: Cuttlefish roe (烏魚蛋)". Translating the Suiyuan Shidan. 2014.


  57. ^ "[Ganoksin] Cuttlefish Casting – Theory and Practice of Goldsmithing". www.ganoksin.com. Retrieved 2016-09-03.


  58. ^ Morris Bywater Limited (2014-02-26), Cuttlefish Casting: The Making of a Gold Signet Ring, retrieved 2016-09-03


  59. ^ Rossiter, Jonathan; Yap, Bryan; Conn, Andrew (2012). "Biomimetic chromatophores for camouflage and soft active surfaces". Bioinspiration & Biomimetics. 7 (3): 036009. Bibcode:2012BiBi....7c6009R. doi:10.1088/1748-3182/7/3/036009. PMID 22549047.


  60. ^ Anthes, Emily (12 September 2012). "Cuttlefish provide smart fashion tips". BBC.com.


  61. ^ Ceph Care | TONMO.com: The Octopus News Magazine Online. TONMO.com. Retrieved on 2015-09-25.



External links






  • YouTube video with examples of cuttlefish color and texture modulations


  • Kings of Camouflage NOVA: Cuttlefish episode on PBS.org

  • The new CEPHBASE within the Encyclopedia of Life (EOL)








Popular posts from this blog

用户:Ww71338ww/绘画

自由群

卑爾根